Lecture 3.
ADJECTIVE
NOUN
The following
features are commonly considered to be characteristic of adjectives.
Meaning:
-
generalized lexico-grammatical primary meaning of
non-temporal property, e.g.: black,
big, clever, etc.;
-
generalized grammatical secondary meaning of
non-temporal property, e.g.: comfortable,
national, graceful, etc.
Combinability with:
-
nouns, mostly in postposition, e.g.:
He
was a pleasant fellow
(T. Mori);
-
verbs in preposition, e.g.:
/ married
young (M. Burgess);
-
adverbs of degree in preposition, e.g.:
...
he
was a deeply emotional man
(S. Sheldon);
-
prepositional combinations in postposition, e.g.:
It is
full of
clean paper (W. Deeping).
Syntactic functions:
-
attributive;
-
predicative.
In attributive function, the
adjective is part of a noun phrase: it generally precedes and modifies the head
noun, e.g.:
She
had a small child in
her arms (W. S. Maugham).
Predicative
adjectives
characterize a noun phrase that is a separate clause element Predicative adjectives
have two syntactic roles: subject predicatives and object predicatives.
Subject
predicatives complement a copular verb, characterizing the noun phrase in
subject position, e.g.:
She was wonderful to
me (D. Robins).
Object
predicatives follow a direct object, making a predication about that noun
phrase, e.g.:
He
made the children happy (R.
Quirk et al.).
Predicative
adjectives are somewhat more frequent in fiction than in the other registers,
In
conversation, attributive and predicative adjectives are both relatively rare
because conversation is more verbal than nominal.
You
got a cold? — No. Just a bit sniffy. Cos I"m — I am cold. And I’ll
be all right
once I’ve warmed up (D. Crystal, D.
Davy).
Morphological structure. As far as their
morphological structure is concerned, adjectives fall under simple,
derived, and с о m p о u n dv
Simple adjectives have neither prefixes nor suffixes, e.
g.: green, high, low, fat, etc.
Derived
adjectives have either a prefix or a suffix or both. Derived
adjectives are usually formed from nouns and verbs. The most productive
adjective-forming suffix is -al, e.g.:
international, local, natural, formal, usual, etc.
The
derivational suffixes -ent, -ive,
and
-ous are
moderately common, too. Cf.:
different,
present, innocent, silent, excellent', active, attractive, expensive, negative,
relative', serious, curious, dangerous, famous, nervous, etc.
The
derivational suffixes -ate, -ful,
and
-less are
relatively rare in all registers. Cf.:
private,
moderate', beautiful, useful', helpless, useless, etc.
Although
the suffixes -like and -type
are
even less common, they have interesting uses. As a matter of fact they retain
the meanings of like and type
as
separate words, and are therefore near the boundary between affixation and
compounding. Cf.:
business-like,
child-like, cat-like, rocklike, sharklike', Hollywood-type, Mr-Smith-type,
textbook-type, etc.
Adjectives
can be derived from other adjectives by the negative prefixes un-,
in-, and non-,
e.g.:
unhappy, inattentive, nonexistent,
etc.
In
the last three examples, prefixation combines with suffixation.
Often,
the difference between the adjective and the participle is not clear-cut. The
verbal force of the participle is explicit for the -ing
form
when a direct object is present, for the -ed
form
— when a by-agent phrase
with a personal agent is present. Cf.:
You
are frightening the children
(ibid).
The
man was offended by the policeman
(ibid).
Compound
adjectives are made from a combination of more than one word
and represent compact, integrated forms of expression, which are not easy to
produce ‘online’ except for lexicalized components, such as tongue-tied,
old-fashioned, etc. No wonder
that compound adjectives are common in the written registers, but are
relatively rare in conversation.
Formally,
compound adjectives take many shapes. Adjectives can be added to other
adjectives, e.g.: grayish-blue.
Compounds
can also be composed of an adjective and a noun, e.g.: full-time.
Many
adjective compounds involve participial forms, e.g.: highly-educated,
good-looking, etc. But
adverb-adjective sequences constitute by far the most productive type of
compound adjectives, especially in news, e.g.: politically-
independent, flercely-competitive, etc.
Reduplicative compounds are more productive in conversation than in the other
registers because they are lexicalized (the two parts rarely occur separately)
and because they serve an emotive purpose (they usually play on sounds), e.g.:
wishy-washy
—
thin and without strength; watery; without determination or clear aims and
principles; roly-poly — fat
and round.
Morphological categories. In
Old English, adjectives were inflected for case, gender, number, and degrees of
comparison. In Modern English, only qualitative adjectives are marked for the
grammatical category of degrees of comparison. Cf.: nice—nicer—nicest]
beautiful—more
beautiful—most beautiful.
According
to their meaning and grammatical characteristics, adjectives can be classified
into qualitative and relative.
Qualitative
adjectives denote qualities of a substance directly, e.g.: small,
brown, quick, etc. Most
qualitative adjectives have degrees of comparison, e.g.: small—smaller—smallest.
From
most qualitative adjectives adverbs can be formed by the suffix -ly,
e.g.:
quick—quickly. Qualitative
adjectives are used both attributively and predicatively. Cf.: What
wonderfully blue eyes you have, Ernest! (O.
Wilde) — attribute. They are quite, quite blue
(ibid)
— subjective predicative.
Relative
adjectives express qualities of a substance through their relation to
materials (wooden),
place
(Italian), time (weekly),
or
action (preparatory),
i.
e. indirectly. Relative adjectives have no degrees of comparison. They do not
form adverbs by the suffix -ly.
Relative
adjectives are chiefly used as attributes, e.g.:
...he
found at the bottom of the box a pair of wooden skates which had been Kate’s
when she was a child (A. J. Cronin).
There
are no hard-and-fast lines between qualitative and relative adjectives. A
relative adjective can acquire the meaning of a qualitative adjective. Cf.:
wooden
walls = walls made of
wood (A. S. Hornby); a wooden smile
=
an inexpressive smile (ibid).
V. N. Zhigadlo, I.
P. Ivanova and L. L. Iofik mention also quantitative adjectives. This class
comprises such words as many, much, little,
and
few. Like
qualitative adjectives, they have degrees of comparison. Cf.: many,
much — more — most, little — less — least,
few —fewer —fewest, e.g.:
As
opposed to qualitative adjectives, which express qualities of an object
directly, and in contrast to relative adjectives, which denote qualities of an
object indirectly, the so-called quantitative adjectives characterize the given
object numerically, just as numerals do. Thus, it is open to discussion whether
many, much, little, few can
be considered adjectives
five
tables —
numeral;
some
tables — pronoun;
many
tables — the so-called quantitative adjective.
Linguistic Status of the Category of Degrees of Comparison
The
problem of degrees of comparison has given rise to much controversy. First of
all, there is no unity of opinion concerning the character of this category in
Modern English. Some linguists think that degrees of comparison should be
treated as a lexical category. In their opinion, long—longer—longest
represent
three different words, not forms of one and the same word.
Criticizing
this point of view, A. I. Smrnitsky says that long—longer—
longest are not
different words, but forms of the same word because they have the same stem long
and
are consequently characterized by identical lexical meaning.
Adjectives
that Lack Degrees of Comparison
As a
rule, only qualitative adjectives admit of degrees of comparison because they
denote properties capable of appearing in different degrees, e.g.: fine—finer—finest.
But
some qualitative adjectives stand outside the category of comparison. They
are:
-
adjectives that express
the highest degree of a quality, e.g.: supreme, extreme,
etc.;
-adjectives
having the suffix -ish,
e.g.:
reddish, whitish, etc.;
-
adjectives with
a negative meaning, e.g.: illiterate
-
Relative adjectives have no degrees of comparison.
Number of Degrees of Comparison
The
next question is how many degrees of comparison the English adjective has. With
qualitative adjectives, which can denote degrees of a given quality, O.Jespersen;
R.Quirk et al give three types of
comparison are possible:
-
comparison to the same
degree;
-
comparison to a lower
degree;
-comparison
to a higher degree [.].
Comparison
in relation to the same degree is
expressed by as ... as,
e.g.:
She's
as pretty as her sister (Longman
Dictionary of Contemporary English).
Comparison
in relation to a lower degree is
expressed by less and least,
e.g.:
H.
Sweet, M. Ganshina and N. Vasilevskaya mention only two degrees of comparison,
namely the comparative degree and the superlative
degree.
However,
there is little justification for excluding the so-called positive degree from
the classification because although it does not imply any comparison, it
forms the basis for comparison. R. Quirk and some other English grammarians
call the positive degree the absolute degree.
The
comparative degree indicates that the quality is found in
the person or thing described in a higher degree than in some other person or thing,
e.g.:
The man in the middle is stronger than
the man on the left (A. S. Hornby).
The
superlative degree denotes the highest degree of a quality,
e.g.: He is the strongest of the three men
(ibid).
Synthetic and Analytical Forms of
Degrees of Comparison
The
problem of forms of degrees of comparison is also controversial. Monosyllabic
adjectives, i. e. adjectives consisting of one syllable, and the disyllabic
adjectives ending in -er, -ly, -le, -y,
and
-w form
the comparative degree by the suffix -er
and
the superlative degree — by the suffix -est.
Cf.:
The
days get longer... (B.
Zaffran, D. Krulik).
...these were the longest days of the year
(J. Cheever).
The examination was easier than we
expected (R. Murphy).
The day we were married was the happiest day of my life (M.
Fuchs, M. Bonner). /
This
is a synthetic way of forming degrees of comparison. The existence of
synthetic forms of degrees of comparison is recognized by the majority of
linguists. As to the combinations with more and most,
less
and least, the question is debatable.
First,
we shall discuss the problem of the combinations with less and least,
e.g.: less important — least important. To
qualify these constructions as analytical degrees of comparison, we must prove
that they represent analytical forms of the adjective important.
Analytical forms are generally opposed to synthetic forms. As to combinations
with less and least,
they
have no parallel synthetic forms to express a lower degree of this or that
quality.
Now
we shall take up the problem of the combinations with more and most,
e.g.:
Let’s talk about something more interesting (R. Murphy).
He’s the most interesting person I’ve ever met (ibid).
According
to V. N. Zhigadlo, I. R Ivanova and L. L. Iofik, they are also outside the
grammatical category of degrees of comparison. First, more
and
most form combinations with adjectives similar
to those with less and least:
more beautiful — less
beautiful, most beautiful — least beautiful, e.g.:
Oh, I’m the most sensible person here — and Lucille is the least sensible (С. E. Eckersley).
Since
the forms less beautiful and least
beautiful are not degrees of comparison, the combinations
with more and most
cannot be considered degrees of comparison either.
Second,
combinations with most can be used
with the indefinite article to express a very high degree of quality, e.g.:
A most tragic thing happened to her early in the war (W.
Deeping).
Finally,
V. N. Zhigadlo, I. P. Ivanova and L. L. Iofik consider it wrong in refer to the
forms with more and most to
degrees of comparison because mare and most
fully retain their lexical meaning. They really do. Cf.: You’ll
be more comfortable if you turn the seat down (С. E.
Ecker- sley).
They were the most beautiful children she had ever seen (S.
Sheldon).
English
grammarians do not use the terms synthetic and analytical forms of degrees of
comparison. They speak of inflectional and phrasal comparison. The essence of
the two sets of terms is the same.
The
choice between inflectional and phrasal comparison, as has already been shown,
is largely determined by the length of the adjective. Although monosyllabic
adjectives normally form the comparative and superlative degrees by inflection,
most monosyllabic and disyllabic adjectives can also form/their degrees of
comparison with more and most.
Phrasal
forms are usually used for emphasis in spoken English. Cf.:
You should be more proud of the things
you’ve already achieved (M. Foley, D.
Hall).
I think this is the one she is the most
proud of (ibid).
In conversation, adjectives are
occasionally doubly marked for degree, carrying both inflectional and phrasal
markers. Cf.:
It’s much more warmer in there
(D. Biber et al.).
She’s a bit more nicer than Mrs. Jones
(ibid).
Irregular
Forms of Comparison
Besides
the already mentioned synthetic and analytical forms of degrees of comparison,
there are irregular forms. A few adjectives have suppletive forms of comparison
that are derived from different roots, e. g.: good—better—best,
bad—worse—worst. Cf.:
Is
Lucille a good dancer? (С. E.
Eckersley).
You’re
a much better cook than your mother was, Elizabeth (S.
Sheldon).
The
best women are divorced... And the best men are married (A.Sillitoe).
A few
adjectives have two sets of degrees of comparison, e.g.: old—older—oldest
(age
in general); old—elder—eldest
(age
within the family). Cf.:
She
is an old woman... (I. Shaw).
His
friends were older than Vivian (S. Sheldon).
...
White’s
was one of the oldest clubs in England...
(ibid).
My
elder brother was in a car accident last week (M.
Swan).
The
eldest daughter does all the housework (ibid).
Since
the second set of forms (elder—
eldest) has a meaning
slightly different from the meaning of the positive degree (old),
they
can hardly be regarded as grammatical forms of degrees of comparison, but
should be qualified as separate lexical units which originally were, perhaps,
grammatical degrees of comparison of the adjective old.
Absolute
Use of the Superlative Degree
Adjectives
in the superlative degree imply limitation, that’s why the noun modified by an
adjective in the superlative degree always combines with the definite article
or one of the definite determiners, e.g.:
It was one of the worst days for him
(N. Hale).
Since
adjectives preceded by the definite article are easily substantivized, the
superlative degree is often used absolutely, either with the head noun
mentioned before or without any noun whatsoever.
Their pool was perhaps the oldest in the country...
(J. Cheever).
But if the worst comes to the worst,
don’t blame me (ibid).
Substantivization
of Adjectives
Adjectives
can be substantivized, i.e. become nouns. When adjectives are converted into
nouns, they no longer indicate properties of substances, but come to express
substances possessing these properties. In I English, the process of
substantivization is easier than in Russian due to I he scantiness of
inflections. Substantivization can be whole and partial.
Adjectives
wholly converted into nouns acquire not only the grammatical meaning of nouns
but also their typical morphological and syntactic characteristics:
-
ability to form the plural,
e.g.:
All
natives have good hands and feet (W. S.
Maugham);
-ability
to be used in the genitive case,
e.g.:
He is
investigating the ancients’ conception of the universe (R.
Quirk et al.);
-ability
to be used with the indefinite article, e.g.:
I spoke
the language like a native... (W. S.
Maugham);
-ability
to be modified by an adjective, e.g.:
My
uncle is my nearest living relative (Longman
Dictionary of Contemporary English);
-the
functions of subject and/or object in
the sentence, e.g.: A native was silently rowing
up-stream... (W. S. Maugham)
— subject;
The
government of the island treated the natives badly (Longman
Dictionary of Contemporary English) — object.
More
often substantivization is but partial. In the case of partial
substantivization, adjectives acquire the grammatical meaning of nouns
(‘thingness’), the noun combinability with the definite article or some other
definite determiner, and the noun functions of subject and/or object, e.g.:
The
poor must stand together everywhere (Th. Dreiser) —
subject. ...it seems to me I saw everybody but the dead
(S. Bellow) — object.
However,
they lack the grammatical categories of case and number and never combine with
the indefinite article.
A. I.
Smirnitsky thinks that in order to become a noun, an adjective must acquire
number distinctions. That’s why he does not recognize partial substantivization
and treats cases like the rich, the poor,
etc.
as the use of adjectives without nouns.
But
the category of number is common only to countable nouns. Uncountable nouns
stand outside the category of number. Nevertheless, nobody denies them the
status of nouns. So, the argument of A. I. Smirnitsky is not convincing.
The
question of adjectivization of nouns presents a number of difficulties, too.
Here we shall deal with such constructions as stone
wall, peace talks, etc.
In
the opinion of B.A. Ilyish, it is practically impossible to prove whether stone in stone
wall is a
noun or an adjective.
H.
Sweet thinks that the first component in these constructions is a noun because
it lacks the category of degrees of comparison. However, many adjectives have
no degrees of comparison either.
E. P.
Shubin also refers the first component in constructions of the type stone
wall to
nouns.
The
divergence of views, in our opinion, is due to the gradual process of
adjectivization. The latter is reflected in dictionaries. Thus, the Longman
Dictionary of Contemporary English presents family and business
as
nouns. For silver it has two
entries:
silver (n) —
a soft whitish precious metal; silver (adj)
— made of silver.
Solid
and hyphenated compounds of the type everyday,
matter-of-fact are qualified there as adjectives.
Only
time will show whether all the attributive nouns will turn into adjectives
proper, but their adjectivization is an indisputable fact.
It
seems reasonable to follow D. Crystal and say that at the present stage nouns,
which appear in the position associated with adjectives, form a ‘mixed’ word
class.
The
first grammarian to mention statives in English was B.A. Ilyish. He thinks that
words of the category of state, for instance, asleep,
alive constitute
a separate part of speech because they possess semantic, morphological, and
syntactic characteristics of their own.
Semantically, he says,
statives are marked by the presence of a seme of state, as opposed to
adjectives that express non-temporal property, e.g.: ...he
had been asleep for some time... (J. K. Jerome), which means that he had
been in a state of sleep for some time.
In
the opinion of L. S. Barkhudarov, the meaning of state is merely a variety of
the meaning of non-temporal property typical of adjectives. So, in his opinion,
statives do not differ from adjectives as far as their meaning is concerned.
Morphologically, statives seem to
stand apart from adjectives, for they have a specific prefix a- and
lack the grammatical category of degrees of comparison. On closer inspection,
the absence of degrees of comparison does not prove anything. On the one hand,
there are a lot of adjectives that stand outside the grammatical category of
degrees of comparison. On the other hand, some of the so-called statives form
degrees of comparison just like most qualitative adjectives, e.g.:
The
two main meals of the day, lunch and dinner, are both more or less alike
(Lingaphone English Course).
As
for the prefix a-, the Longman
Dictionary of Contemporary English regards it as an
adjective-forming prefix.
A.
A. Ilyish thinks that statives possess
the category of tense. He is asleep, in
his opinion, refers to the present tense as opposed to He
was asleep,
which
is past, and to He will be asleep, which
is future.
However,
this point of view does not seem convincing.
The combinability of statives and
adjectives, according to L.S. Barkhudarov, is also alike. Thus, both
adjectives and statives can be modified by adverbs and prepositional
combinations. Cf.:
She
was very happy (W.
S. Maugham).
In a
minute she was fast asleep
(P. Abrahams).
He was
conscious of a sense of adventure
(W. Deeping).
Yet
he was aware of a sense of unreality
(ibid).
The
only thing that differentiates statives from adjectives is their syndetic
function in the sentence. Adjectives are generally used both attributively and
predicatively, statives — mainly predicatively:
She
is aloof from her classmates (S. Sheldon).
...his
soul was ablaze with bliss (M. Twain).
I was
aware again of that feeling of discomfort (D.
du Maurier).
That’s
why we shall look upon words with the prefix a- as a
specific subclass of adjectives.
The English noun is a part of speech that is
characterized by the following features.
Meaning:
-
generalized
lexico-grammatical primary meaning of Thingness5, e.g.: table,
chair, lamp, etc,;
-generalized grammatical secondary meaning of
Thingness5, e.g.: joy, peace, milk, etc.
Combinability with:
-
verbs, both in
preposition and in postposition, e.g.:
He closed the door... (S. Sheldon).
The door closed
(I. Murdoch);
-
adjectives,
usually in preposition, e.g.:
She was a beautiful girl... (J. Cheever);
-
prepositive nouns,
both in the common and in the genitive case, e. g.:
I saw it in the Chicago newspaper (F. S. Fitzgerald).
... and the evenings were long and happy, because
Robert’s father was there (N.Hale);
-
prepositive articles and other determiners,
e.g.:
Wait
a minute (Е/Hemingway).
The father tried his best...
(W. C. Williams).
Mv heart sank a little
(W. S. Maugham);
-
prepositions, e.g.:
He read a letter from his wife
to me (T. Mori).
Syntactic functions:
-
subject, e.g.:
Father decided to take a
holiday from his office... (S.
Leacock);
-object,
e.g.:
You love your parents, don’t
you? (J. D. Salinger).
Morphological structure. As far as
their morphological structure is concerned, nouns fall under the following
types: simple,
derived, and compound.
Simple
nouns have neither prefixes nor suffixes, e.g.: book,
pen, pencil, table, chair, lamp, etc.
Derived
nouns have either a prefix or a suffix or both. Noun derivational prefixes
typically do not change the word class; i.e. the prefix is attached to a noun
base to form a new noun with a different meaning, e.g.:
group — subgroup.
Noun
derivational suffixes, on the other hand, often change the word class; i. e.
the suffix is often attached to a verb or adjective base to form a noun with a
different meaning. Cf.: agree
(v) — agreement
(n); effective (adj) — effectiveness
(n).
There
are, however, also many nouns that are derived by suffixes from other nouns,
e.g.:
infant
(n) — infancy
(n).
In
compound nouns two or more than two words are combined to form a single noun.
In English, compounding is a highly productive process. Cf.: eye-witness,
lamp-post, bigwig, cookbook, rocking-chair, income, self-control,
etc.
Practice
varies as to whether to represent a compound as two orthographic words, one
unbroken orthographic word, or a hyphenated word. Partly this is because there
is no clear dividing line between compounds and free combinations.
Compounds
are over twice as frequent in news than in conversation. It is not surprising,
for the overall frequency of nouns in conversation is much smaller than in
news. What is more, the greater variety of compound patterns in news fits in
with the tendency of this register to use a more varied vocabulary.
‘Noun
+ noun compounds’ are the
most productive type structurally, e.g.: newspaper.
The next most common types of compounds are those consisting of ‘adjective
+ noun’ and those beginning or ending with a particle,
e.g.: highway, feedback, outfit.
Morphological categories.
Nouns have the morphological categories of case and number.
Category of Case Definition of Case
The
notion of case goes back to Ancient Greece.
Nowadays,
case is usually regarded as a morphological form of a declinable word used to
Express a certain meaning or to denote a certain relation to other words [С. T.
Onions],
B.
A. Ilyish gives another definition. In his opinion, case is a category of the
noun expressing relations between the thing denoted by the noun and another
thing, property, or action. This definition does not stand criticism: being a
linguistic notion, case cannot connect objects of extra linguistic reality.
Number of Cases
Linguists
are still at variance as to the number of cases in Modem English.
Representatives of universal grammar speak of 6 cases.
J.C.Nesfield
mentions 5 cases: nominative, vocative, accusative, genitive, and dative
remarking at the same time that the genitive is the only case that is now
indicated by change of form. The other cases have lost their case inflections
and are indicated only by grammatical relation.
When
a noun is used as subject, it is said to be in the nominative case, e.g.:
Rain
falls (J. C. Nesfield).
When
a noun is used for the sake of address, it is said to be in the vocative case,
e.g.:
Are you coming, my friend?
(ibid).
When
a noun is a direct object, it is said to be in the accusative case, e.g.:
Mary
took the money (M. Vince, K. McNicholas).
When
a noun is an indirect object, it is said to be in the dative case, e.g.:
I gave the boy a penny
(J. C. Nesfield).
According
to G. Curme, there are 4 cases in Modern English: nominative, genitive,
dative, and accusative. These cases did exist in Old English. In the course of
time, however, the original nominative, dative, and accusative coincided in one
form that is opposed nowadays to the inflected genitive.
Every grammatical category should comprise a
limited number of members. If we referred prepositional combinations to case
forms, the number of cases would grow immensely, and we would be merely creating
the illusion of classification.
The
majority of linguists recognize the existence of 2 cases in Modern English:
common and genitive. The common case is unmarked both in meaning and in form.
It has a very general meaning that is specified by means of word order and prepositions
and that may be characterized only negatively as a non-genitive form. It is
represented by a zero exponent. Nouns in the common case can perform any
syntactic function in the sentence. Cf.:
Suddenly the weather changed
(L. Untermeyer) — subject.
He
touched my hand (G. Jones) — direct object.
The
grocer gave the baby a stick of candy... (H.
Garland) — non- prepositional indirect object.
Mrs.
Hall did not ask about her affairs (H.
Garland) — prepositional indirect object.
He
was a shy man (B. MacLeverty) — predicative.
She’s
in the souvenir shop (English Course) — attribute.
He had not seen Mabel for
seven years (W. S. Maugham) — adverbial.
Genitive
Case
Form of the Genitive Case
The
genitive case is marked both in meaning and in form. The regular way of
forming the genitive case of singular nouns is by adding’s,
e.g.: My sister's little girl fell
downstairs (J. Cheever).
There
are two ways of forming the genitive case of plural nouns. If the plural ends
in -s, we just add an apostrophe,
e.g.:
Even grandmothers'
dreams don't always come true... (D.H. Lawrence).
If the plural does not end in -s, we add's,
e.g.:
The children's
toys are new (R. Quirk et ah).
Meaning of the Genitive Case
The
central meaning of the genitive case is that of possession, e.g.: Vinny
would inherit her mother’s money (D. H.
Lawrence).
That’s
why A. I: Smirnitsky suggests that the genitive case should be called the
possessive case.
Types of Nouns Used in the
Genitive Case
In
Old English, the genitive case was freely formed from all nouns. In Modern
English, the genitive case is restricted to the following nouns: -personal names,
e.g.:
George Washington's statue
(R. Quirk et al.);
-personal nouns,e.g.: the
boy's new bicycle (ibid);
-animal nouns,
in particular those denoting 'higher animals’, e.g.:
the horse's tail
(ibid); the dog's collar (ibid);
-collective nouns,
which emphasize the aspect of 'organized individuals’, in particular those
denoting authoritative and other organizational bodies, e.g.:
the government's economic
plans (ibid); the committee's decision
(ibid);
-geographical names:
continents:
Europe'sfuture (ibid);
countries: China's development
(ibid); states: Maryland's Democratic senator
(ibid); cities and towns: London's
water supply (ibid); universities: Harvard's
Department of Linguistics (ibid); -locative nouns
denoting regions, institutions, heavenly bodies, etc. They can be very similar
to geographical names, and are often written with initial capital letters,
e.g.: the world's economy
(ibid); the Club's pianist
(ibid); the hotel's entrance
(ibid); the school's history
(ibid);
-temporal nouns,e.g.: a
day's work (ibid); a
moment's thought (ibid); today's
paper (ibid);
I |
Use of the Genitive Case
As
to its use, the genitive case falls under dependent and independent. Dependent
genitives are used with the nouns they modify and come before them, e.g.:
He stared at his aunt’s face
(J. C. Oates).
Independent
genitives occur without a following head noun. Many independent genitives
involve ellipsis. In elliptical genitives, the head noun can be inferred either
from the preceding or from the following context. Cf.:
My car is faster than John’s
(R. Quirk et al.).
Mary’s was the prettiest dress
(ibid).
Choice between S-Genitives and Of-Phrases
The
genitive meaning can be rendered by a noun as head of a prepositional phrase
with of. The of-phrase is normally
used with inanimate concrete nouns, e.g.:
the roof of this house
(R. Quirk et al.).
The
choice between the s-genitive and the of-phrase, according to the authors of the Longman
Grammar of Spoken and Written English, varies
depending upon a number of factors, the most important of which are: register,
the type of dependent noun, semantic relations between head and the dependent
phrase, the complexity of the dependent phrase, the information status of the
dependent phrase, and specific collocations.
Sometimes
the genitive suffix is attached not to the head noun, but lo the last word of a
genitive phrase. It is the so-called group genitive. The group genitive is most
common with of-phrases and coordinate phrases. Cf.:
the Museum of Modem Art’s
Director (R. Quirk et al.); a minute or two’s
rest (D. Biber et al.).
From
a logical point of view, the distinction is between one and more than one. The
corresponding grammatical distinction is between singular and plural, e.g.:
a table—tables.
Some
linguists say that the essential meaning of the category of number is not that
of quantity, but that of discreteness. The plural, according to them, denotes
something consisting of distinguishable parts, e.g.: spectacles,
scissors, trousers, etc. [E. A. Korneyeva, N. A.
Kobrina, K. A. Guzeyeva, M.I.Ossovskayaj.
These
nouns do indicate discrete things consisting of two parts. But we are hardly
justified in referring them to the plural number because they have no singular
counterparts, and the plural and the singular are correlative notions: when
there is no singular, we cannot speak about the plural, and vice versa.
So,
the generalized grammatical meaning of number is that of quantity. In Modern
English, it is represented by the opposition ‘oneness (singular) — more than
oneness (plural)’.
At
first sight, it may seem that the difference between the singular and the
plural is not grammatical, but lexical since, for example, table
(singular) and tables
(plural) denote different objects of extra linguistic reality [F. F.
Fortunatov]
However,
we know that the meaning of a word cannot be identified with the thing it is
used to denote. Besides, we should not disregard the fact that the idea of
plurality usually has constant grammatical forms of its expression. In English,
it is the inflection -(e)s,
e.g.: a lamp
— lamps, a box—boxes.
(The
inflection -es is added after -s, -ss, -ch, -sh,
-tch, -x, -z, and -o.)
We
can only speak of ‘more than one’, i. e. of the plural, in regard to things,
which, without being identical, belong to the same kind [O. Jespersen].
Plurality, thus, presupposes difference, but if the difference is too great,
it is impossible to use words like ‘two’ or ‘three’. For instance, a brick and
a musical sound are not two.
Some
linguists single out two other types of the plural: lexicalized plural and the
plural of approximation.
The
so-called lexicalized plural either introduces new shades of meaning into the
singular or comes to render a totally different meaning. Cf.: Tragedy
is lack of experience (D. H. Lawrence).
He’s had many odd experiences
(R. Quirk et al.).
Colour (цвет) — colours (флаг).
The
form of lexicalized plural is identical with that of grammatical plural: -(e)s.
But the meaning of lexicalized plural is always different from the
corresponding singular. That’s why it should be excluded from the grammatical
category of number, for the components of the grammatical category of number
should be lexically identical.
The plural of approximation, mentioned by O.
Jespersen, in our opinion, is closer to lexical forms, for though, like
grammatical plural, it ends in -(e)s and denotes several objects,
the objects do not belong to the same kind, e.g.: There are many things
people remember about the sixties (J. C. Richards, J. Hull, S. Proctor),
where sixties does not mean 'one sixty + another sixty + ...’,
but ‘sixty’ + ‘sixty-one’ + ‘sixty-two’, and so on till ‘sixty-nine’.
There are several irregular ways of forming the
plural:
-voicing of final
consonants--splural. Some nouns ending in -/or
-fe form their plurals by changing the ending to -ves, e.g.i
a knife— knives.
Others have regular plurals as well, e.g.: a
scarf—scarves (scarfs);
-mutation plurals. In a few nouns, the plural is formed by mutation,
i.e. a change in the vowel, e.g.: a man —men; a woman — women; a foot—feet;
a tooth —teeth, etc.;
- -en plurals, e.g.: an ox—oxen.
Children,
the plural of child, combines a vowel
change and the irregular ending -en;
-zero plurals. Countable nouns that have the same form for singular
and plural are said to have zero plural, e.g.: a sheep—sheep; a deer—deer;
-foreign plurals. In many learned words scholars have introduced the
plural as well as the singular form from foreign languages, e. g.:
curriculum
— curricula] formula — formulae.
There is, however, a strong tendency to inflect such
words in the English way, especially in everyday speech, e.g.: a
formula—formulas.
As regards the category of number, all English nouns
can be divided into two classes: countable and uncountable. Countable nouns are
those that have the opposition ‘singular—plural’, e.g.: a book—books.
Uncountable nouns do not call up the idea of any
definite thing with a certain shape or precise limits. They are either
material, e.g.: silver, water, butter, gas, etc., or abstract, e.g.: leisure,
music, success, tact, etc.
Singular collective nouns that refer to groups of
people (e.g.: family, team, government, etc.) may be treated as either
singular or plural. They are treated as plural, especially in British English,
when the focus is on the group as individuals. In these cases, a plural verb is
used, and the group is referred to by the pronouns they and who,
e.g.:
My family arg wonderful. They do all they can
for me. I don't know any other family who would do so much (M. Swan).
They are treated as singular when we see the group as
an impersonal unit. In these cases, a singular verb is used, and the group is
referred to by the pronoun it ^nd the words which and that,
not who, e.g.:
The average family (which now consists
of four members at most) is a great deal smaller than й used to be (ibid).
In American English, singular verbs are normal with
most of collective nouns (though the noun family can have a plural
verb).
In British English, they prefer to use a singular verb
if it combines with the indefinite rather than the definite article.
Those uncountable nouns that always combine with
plural verbs and are substituted by plural pronouns are called Pluralia Tantum.
Most Pluralia Tantum end in -s. Cf.:
My trousers are getting too small round the waist (ibid).
The nurse’s wages were good (W. Collins).
Where arg my scissors?
(A. S. Hornby).
The sugar-tongs were too wide for one of her
hands, and she had to use both in wielding them (Ch. Bronte).
Some Pluralia' Tantum lack the final -s. They
include the following nouns:
-
people, e.g.:
Were
there many people at the meeting? (Longmap
Dictionary of Contemporary English);
-police, e.g.:
The police have caught the murderer (ibid);
-
cattle, e.g.:
All his cattle were grazing in the field (R. Quirk et ah);
-
poultry (farmyard birds), e.g.:
Where are your poultry?
(ibid)[1];
-
livestock (animals kept on a farm), e.g.:
Our livestock arg not as numerous as they used
to be (ibid);
-
vermin, e.g.:
These
vermin cause disease (ibid).
Traditionally, gender is defined as a morphological
category that finds its expression in special noun inflections of gender and
that is closely tied I о the sex of the referent.
There is no unity of opinion concerning the category
of gender in Modern English. Old English nouns distinguished 3 grammatical genders:
masculine, feminine, and neuter. H. Sweet finds the same 3 genders in Modern
English.
Criticizing the conception of H. Sweet, A.I.Smirnitsky
emphasizes that in Modern English it is not nouns, but the things they denote
that are classified into the so-called genders. For instance, there is no
formal difference between the nouns boy and girl. But the noun boy
is considered to belong to the masculine gender, the noun girl — to the
feminine gender. In other words, gender in Modem English nouns is expressed
lexically, by using:
-
totally different nouns, e.g.: father—mother,
son — daughter, uncle—aunt,
man —woman', bull—cow, etc;
-derived nouns with masculine and feminine suffixes: -erj-or,
-ess, e.g.:
waiter— waitress', actor—actress',
-
compound nouns in -man and -woman, e.g.: policeman —policewoman',
-
a modifier denoting sex, e.g.: boy-friend-girl-friend',
he-goat—she-goaf,
Tom-cat—Pussy-cat, male nurse; female officer, woman doctor, etc.
English speakers use masculine terms more often than
feminine terms. There are two reasons for the preference of male terms over
female terms.
Gender
in Modern English is expressed neither morphologically i. e. by special
inflections of gender, nor syntactically, i. e. by forms o; agreement. Gender
in Modern English is a purely lexical category.
Semantic Classification of Nouns
The
semantic classification of nouns still causes much controversy among linguists.
According to W. L. Chafe, the distinction between countable and uncountable
nouns is most important. V. V. Bogdanov takes the opposition ‘animate
—inanimate’ as a starting point for his noun classification. We side with Y.
S. Stepanov in regarding the relation to extra linguistic reality as a basis
for a semantic classification of nouns.
At
the first stage, in accordance with the presence or absence of direct
connection with extra linguistic reality, all nouns are divided into those
denoting objects and those denoting non-objects, i.e. abstract notions. Objects
are further subdivided into those having clear-cut boundaries and those having
no definite boundaries, i.e. material nouns, e.g.: water, milk, sand,
etc. Objects having clear-cut boundaries fall into living and non-living, i.e.
things, e.g.:pen,
table, chair, etc. Living objects can be animate and
inanimate, i.e. plants, e.g.: rose, tulip, lily, etc. Animate
living objects either lack person characteristics (animals), e.g.: cat, dog, fox,
etc. or possess person characteristics (people).
Nouns
denoting things, people, and sometimes plants and materials can be classified
into two large classes: common, e.g.:pen, cat, boy, rose, water,
etc. and proper, e.g.: Britain, Rex, John, Burgundy, etc.
Common nouns generally draw a distinction between singularity and plurality.
Cf.:
This is a hat.
— These are hats
(A. S. Hornby).
This is a child.
— These are children
(С. E. Eckersley).
The
only exception is constituted by common material nouns, where the plural
suffix, as a rule, introduces a new shade of meaning that is incompatible with
the grammatical plural. Cf.:
The water feels
very cold on winter mornings... (С. E. Eckersley). Where are we
going, Grandpa? — To wash in the waters of bitterness (A. J.
Cronin).
In
common nouns denoting people and sometimes animals, the dichotomy
‘singularity—plurality’ is supplemented by collective nouns, e.g.: people, police, cattle,
etc. Collective nouns denoting people, in the opinion of R. Quirk and his
co-authors, possess person characteristics when they combine with plural verbs
and/or are substituted by plural pronouns, e.g.:
The committee have met and they have rejected the proposal (R. Quirk et al.).
Комментариев нет:
Отправить комментарий